职称英语理工B级阅读理解模拟题及答案(五)
2018年09月26日 来源:来学网来学网小编为广大考生整理了2018职称英语模拟试题,更多资讯信息敬请关注来学网!
Driven to Distraction
Joe Coyne slides into the driver’s eat, starts up the car and heads to town. The empty stretch of interstate gives way to urban congestion, and Coyne hits the brakes as a pedestrian suddenly crosses the street in front of him.
But even if he hadn’t stopped in time, the woman would have been safe. She isn’t real. Neither is the town. And Coyne isn’t really driving. Coyne is demonstrating a computerized driving simulator that is helping researchers at Old Dominion University (ODU) examine how in-vehicle guidance systems affect the person behind the wheel.
The researchers want to know if such systems, which give audible or written directions, are too distracting—or whether any distractions are offset by the benefits drivers get from having help finding their way in unfamiliar locations.
“We’re looking at the performance and mental workload of drivers,” said Caryl Baldwin, the assistant psychology professor leading the research, which involves measuring drivers’ reaction time and brain activity as they respond to auditory and visual cues.
The researchers just completed a study of the mental workload involved in driving through different kinds of environments and heavy vs. light traffic. Preliminary results show that as people “get into more challenging driving situations, they don’t have any extra mental energy to respond to something else in the environment,” Baldwin said.
But the tradeoffs could be worth it, she said. This next step is to test different ways of giving drivers navigational information and how those methods change the drivers’ mental workload.
“Is it best if they see a picture…that shows their position, a map kind of display?” Baldwin said. “Is it best if they hear it?” navigational systems now on the market give point-by-point directions that follow a prescribed route. “They’re very unforgiving,” Baldwin said. “If you miss a turn, they can almost seem to get angry.”
That style of directions also can be frustrating for people who prefer more general instructions. But such broad directions can confuse drivers who prefer route directions, Baldwin said.
Perhaps manufacturers should allow drivers to choose the style of directions they want, or modify systems to present some information in a way that makes sense for people who prefer the survey style, she said.
Interestingly, other research has shown that about 60 percent of men prefer the survey style, while 60 percent women prefer the route style, Baldwin said. This explains the classic little thing of why men don’t like to stop and ask for directions and women do, Baldwin added.
1. Which statement is true of the description in the first two paragraphs?
A. If Coyne had stopped the car in time, he wouldn’t have hit the woman.
B. The woman would have been knocked over, if Coyne had followed the traffic regulations.
C. Coyne is not really driving so it is impossible for him to have hit the woman.
D. If the woman had not crossed the street suddenly, Coyne would not have hit her.
2. What do researchers want to find out, according to the third and fourth paragraphs?
A. Whether or not audible or written directions are distracting.
B. how long it will take the driver to respond to auditory and visual stimuli.
C. How the driver perform under certain metal workload.
D. All of the above.
3. What are the preliminary results given in the fifth paragraph?
A. Drivers are afraid of getting into challenging driving situations.
B. In challenging driving situations, drivers still have extra energy to handle other things.
C. In challenging driving situations, drivers do not have any additional mental energy to deal with something else.
D. Drivers’ mental load remains unchanged under different situations.
4. The sixth paragraph mainly state that the researchers.
A. is designing a visual navigational information system.
B. is designing an audio navigational information system.
C. is designing an audio-visual navigational information system.
D. want to determine the best ways of giving navigational information system.
5. What kind of directions do men and women prefer?
A. Women prefer more general directions and men prefer route directions.
B. Men prefer more general directions and women prefer route direction.
C. Both men and women prefer general directions.
D. Both men and women prefer route directions.
答案与解释 :
1. C 根据第一段和第二段的内容,读者可以知道,这不是 Coyne真实的驾车经历。第二段的第一句是虚拟语气,意思是即使他没有及时刹车,那位妇女也是安全的。因此 A、B和 D都不符合句意。
2. D 第三段告诉我们,研究者要了解什么样的驾车指南会使回车者分心。第四段告诉我们,他们要研究驾车者在驾驶中的精神负荷,测试驾车者对声音和图像的反应,包括反映时间和大脑活动。所以, D是正确选项。
3. C第五段昀后一句提供了答案。
4. D 根据本段第一句可以得知答案。
5.B 文章的昀后四段讨论驾车指南的两种类型:第九段使用的两个表达是: general instructions和 route directions 即是第八段中的 point-by-point directions that follow a prescribed route;第十段和第十一段使用的表达是:survey style 和 route style。因此, general instructions或 general directions指的是一种传递总体信息的驾车指南, point-by-point directions和 route style是一种传递具体路线信息的驾车指南。根据昀后一段的描述,大多数男士偏向于 general directions,而女士则偏向于 point-by-point directions,即 route style。
译文:
分散注意力驾驶
JoeCoyne滑进驾驶室,发动汽车朝城里开去。空荡荡的那段州际公路结束了,进入到拥塞的城市。这时,一个行人突然从Coyne的车前穿过,他急忙紧急刹车。
但是,就算Coyne来不及刹车,那个妇女也不会有事儿。因为,她是一个假人。整座城市也是假的。Coyne并不是真的在开车。他只是在演示一个计算机操控的驾驶模拟器,帮助OldDominion大学的研究者们检测车内导向系统如何影响开车人。
研究者们希望了解驾驶员在陌生环境里从这一系统提供的那些语音或书面的说明中得到的导路指南等益处是否抵消了这些东西引起的注意力不集中的问题。
主持研究的心理学副教授CarylBaldwin说:“我们一直关注着驾驶员的表现和精神负荷”这包括驾驶员在对听觉和视觉提示做出反应时的反应时间和大脑活动。
研究人员刚刚完成了一项关于在不同环境中,如交通畅通或交通拥挤时驾驶员精神负荷的调查。Baldwin说,初步的调查结果显示人们“在更富有挑战性的环境中驾驶时,并不会对周围环境的变化做出更大的反应。”
她说,两种提示的交替使用还是有效的。下一步,他们将测试为驾驶员提供导向信息的不同方法以及这些方法如何改变驾驶员的精神负荷。
Baldwin说:“是给驾驶员看类似地图那样的显示图片好,还是让他们听到指示信息好呢?”
现在市场上的导向系统会给出点对点的方向信息,同时还会提供预定的路线。Baldwin说:“这些系统通常不会原谅人的错误。如果驾驶员错过了一个转变,它们就会变得非常生气。”
这种提供方向信息的方式通常会使更喜欢笼统信息的驾驶员产生一种受挫感。Baldwin说,笼统的信息却会使更喜欢线路批示的驾驶员感到困惑。
她说,也许,是系统制造商们应该允许驾驶员能够选择自己喜欢的指示方式,或者使系统能够为更喜欢调查信息方式的驾驶员提供有用的信息。
有意思的是,其他研究者表示60%的男性更喜欢这种提供调查信息的导向系统,而60%的女性则更喜欢线路指示系统。Baldwin说,这也就可以解释那个为什么女人喜欢下车问路,而男人却不的经典例子。
小编:二月